Sleep spindle maturity promotes slow oscillation-spindle coupling across child and adolescent development.


Journal

eLife
ISSN: 2050-084X
Titre abrégé: Elife
Pays: England
ID NLM: 101579614

Informations de publication

Date de publication:
24 Nov 2023
Historique:
received: 19 09 2022
accepted: 18 10 2023
medline: 27 11 2023
pubmed: 24 11 2023
entrez: 24 11 2023
Statut: epublish

Résumé

The synchronization of canonical fast sleep spindle activity (12.5-16 Hz, adult-like) precisely during the slow oscillation (0.5-1 Hz) up peak is considered an essential feature of adult non-rapid eye movement sleep. However, there is little knowledge on how this well-known coalescence between slow oscillations and sleep spindles develops. Leveraging individualized detection of single events, we first provide a detailed cross-sectional characterization of age-specific patterns of slow and fast sleep spindles, slow oscillations, and their coupling in children and adolescents aged 5-6, 8-11, and 14-18 years, and an adult sample of 20- to 26-year-olds. Critically, based on this, we then investigated how spindle and slow oscillation maturity substantiate age-related differences in their precise orchestration. While the predominant type of fast spindles was development-specific in that it was still nested in a frequency range below the canonical fast spindle range for the majority of children, the well-known slow oscillation-spindle coupling pattern was evident for sleep spindles in the adult-like canonical fast spindle range in all four age groups-but notably less precise in children. To corroborate these findings, we linked personalized measures of fast spindle maturity, which indicate the similarity between the prevailing development-specific and adult-like canonical fast spindles, and slow oscillation maturity, which reflects the extent to which slow oscillations show frontal dominance, with individual slow oscillation-spindle coupling patterns. Importantly, we found that fast spindle maturity was uniquely associated with enhanced slow oscillation-spindle coupling strength and temporal precision across the four age groups. Taken together, our results suggest that the increasing ability to generate adult-like canonical fast sleep spindles actuates precise slow oscillation-spindle coupling patterns from childhood through adolescence and into young adulthood. Cells in the brain are wired together like an electric circuit that can relay information from one area of the brain to the next. Even when sleeping, the human brain continues to send signals to process information it has encountered during the day. This results in two patterns of electrical activity that define the sleeping brain: slowly repeating waves (or slow oscillations) and rapid bursts of activity known as sleep spindles. Although slow oscillations and sleep spindles are generated in different regions of the brain, they often happen at the same time. This syncing of activity is thought to help different parts of the brain to communicate with each other. Such communication is essential for new memories to become stable and last a long time. In children, slow oscillations and sleep spindles appear together less frequently, suggesting that these co-occurring patterns of electrical activity develop as humans grow into adults. Here, Joechner et al. set out to understand what drives slow oscillations and sleep spindles to start happening at the same time. The team used a technique called electroencephalography (or EEG for short) to study the brain activity of children, teenagers and adults as they slept. This revealed that slow oscillations and sleep spindles occur together less often in children compared to teenagers and adults. Moreover, the slow oscillations and sleep spindles observed in the children had very different physical characteristics to those observed in adults. Further analyses showed that the more similar the children’s sleep spindles were to adult spindles, the more consistently they appeared at the same time as the slow oscillations. The findings of Joechner et al. suggest that as children grow up, their sleep spindles become more adult-like, causing the spindles to happen at the same time as slow oscillations more consistently. This indicates that brain circuits that generate sleep spindles may play an essential role in developing successful communication networks in the human brain. In the future, this work may ultimately provide new insights into how age-related changes to the brain contribute to cognitive development, and suggests sleep as a potential intervention target for neurodevelopmental disorders.

Autres résumés

Type: plain-language-summary (eng)
Cells in the brain are wired together like an electric circuit that can relay information from one area of the brain to the next. Even when sleeping, the human brain continues to send signals to process information it has encountered during the day. This results in two patterns of electrical activity that define the sleeping brain: slowly repeating waves (or slow oscillations) and rapid bursts of activity known as sleep spindles. Although slow oscillations and sleep spindles are generated in different regions of the brain, they often happen at the same time. This syncing of activity is thought to help different parts of the brain to communicate with each other. Such communication is essential for new memories to become stable and last a long time. In children, slow oscillations and sleep spindles appear together less frequently, suggesting that these co-occurring patterns of electrical activity develop as humans grow into adults. Here, Joechner et al. set out to understand what drives slow oscillations and sleep spindles to start happening at the same time. The team used a technique called electroencephalography (or EEG for short) to study the brain activity of children, teenagers and adults as they slept. This revealed that slow oscillations and sleep spindles occur together less often in children compared to teenagers and adults. Moreover, the slow oscillations and sleep spindles observed in the children had very different physical characteristics to those observed in adults. Further analyses showed that the more similar the children’s sleep spindles were to adult spindles, the more consistently they appeared at the same time as the slow oscillations. The findings of Joechner et al. suggest that as children grow up, their sleep spindles become more adult-like, causing the spindles to happen at the same time as slow oscillations more consistently. This indicates that brain circuits that generate sleep spindles may play an essential role in developing successful communication networks in the human brain. In the future, this work may ultimately provide new insights into how age-related changes to the brain contribute to cognitive development, and suggests sleep as a potential intervention target for neurodevelopmental disorders.

Identifiants

pubmed: 37999945
doi: 10.7554/eLife.83565
pii: 83565
pmc: PMC10672804
doi:
pii:

Types de publication

Journal Article

Langues

eng

Sous-ensembles de citation

IM

Subventions

Organisme : Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft
ID : WE 4269/5-1
Organisme : Jacobs Foundation
ID : Early Career Research Fellowship 2017-2019
Organisme : Austrian Science Fund
ID : T397-B02
Organisme : Jacobs Foundation
ID : JS1112H
Organisme : Austrian Science Fund
ID : W1233-G17
Organisme : Austrian Science Fund
ID : P25000
Organisme : German Academic Exchange Service
ID : PRIME Fellowship

Informations de copyright

© 2023, Joechner et al.

Déclaration de conflit d'intérêts

AJ, MH, GG, KH, MW No competing interests declared

Références

Hum Brain Mapp. 2022 Sep;43(13):3923-3943
pubmed: 35488512
Clin Electroencephalogr. 1999 Apr;30(2):39-43
pubmed: 10358781
Clin Neurophysiol. 2019 Jun;130(6):893-902
pubmed: 30981174
Hippocampus. 2015 Oct;25(10):1073-188
pubmed: 26135716
Physiol Rev. 2020 Apr 1;100(2):805-868
pubmed: 31804897
Sleep. 2011 Oct 01;34(10):1411-21
pubmed: 21966073
Curr Opin Physiol. 2020 Jun;15:172-182
pubmed: 32455180
Sci Rep. 2020 Jul 24;10(1):12746
pubmed: 32704003
J Neurosci. 2013 Jan 2;33(1):227-33
pubmed: 23283336
Sleep. 2016 Jan 01;39(1):193-201
pubmed: 26285004
Neuroscience. 2006;137(4):1087-106
pubmed: 16343791
Front Neurosci. 2022 Jun 23;16:891523
pubmed: 35812209
Neuron. 2010 May 13;66(3):353-69
pubmed: 20471349
J Neurosci. 2021 May 12;41(19):4253-4261
pubmed: 33785642
Sci Rep. 2019 Feb 13;9(1):1940
pubmed: 30760741
BMJ. 1995 Jan 21;310(6973):170
pubmed: 7833759
Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010 Feb;11(2):114-26
pubmed: 20046194
Psychophysiology. 2020 Mar;57(3):e13523
pubmed: 31930523
Physiol Rev. 2013 Apr;93(2):681-766
pubmed: 23589831
Neuropsychol Rev. 2010 Jun;20(2):209-25
pubmed: 20422451
Brain Commun. 2020 Jun 13;2(2):fcaa071
pubmed: 32954326
Nat Neurosci. 2016 Jul;19(7):959-64
pubmed: 27182818
Neuron. 1998 Nov;21(5):1123-8
pubmed: 9856467
Front Hum Neurosci. 2015 Jul 21;9:415
pubmed: 26257631
J Neurosci. 1986 Aug;6(8):2134-45
pubmed: 3746404
J Neurosci. 2011 Jun 22;31(25):9124-34
pubmed: 21697364
Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011;2011:156869
pubmed: 21253357
Nat Neurosci. 2015 Nov;18(11):1679-1686
pubmed: 26389842
Sleep. 2010 Apr;33(4):475-80
pubmed: 20394316
Dev Sci. 2019 Jan;22(1):e12706
pubmed: 30252185
Neuron. 2018 Jan 3;97(1):221-230.e4
pubmed: 29249289
Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2015 Apr;31:51-61
pubmed: 25212583
Sleep Med. 2018 Feb;42:73-82
pubmed: 29458750
J Neurosci. 2011 Jul 13;31(28):10331-9
pubmed: 21753010
Elife. 2020 Jun 24;9:
pubmed: 32579108
Neuroimage. 2016 Jul 1;134:607-616
pubmed: 27103135
J Physiol. 1996 Jul 1;494 ( Pt 1):251-64
pubmed: 8814619
Sleep. 2014 Sep 01;37(9):1501-12
pubmed: 25142558
Cereb Cortex. 2011 Mar;21(3):607-15
pubmed: 20624840
Nat Commun. 2019 Aug 8;10(1):3572
pubmed: 31395890
J Neurosci. 1993 Aug;13(8):3266-83
pubmed: 8340807
Elife. 2023 Nov 24;12:
pubmed: 37999945
Front Syst Neurosci. 2010 May 18;4:10
pubmed: 20514143
Nat Commun. 2021 May 25;12(1):3112
pubmed: 34035303
Sleep Med Rev. 2003 Oct;7(5):423-40
pubmed: 14573378
J Neurosci. 2017 Nov 29;37(48):11675-11687
pubmed: 29084867
J Neurosci. 2002 Dec 15;22(24):10941-7
pubmed: 12486189
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003 Feb 18;100(4):2065-9
pubmed: 12576550
Sleep. 2021 Jun 11;44(6):
pubmed: 33367905
Front Hum Neurosci. 2017 Sep 05;11:433
pubmed: 28928647
PLoS One. 2015 Dec 15;10(12):e0144720
pubmed: 26671283
J Neurosci. 2020 Feb 26;40(9):1909-1919
pubmed: 31959699
Neuroimage. 2016 Jul 1;134:213-222
pubmed: 27039143
Psychophysiology. 2013 Jun;50(6):570-82
pubmed: 23551082
Brain. 2007 Nov;130(Pt 11):2868-78
pubmed: 17615093
J Neurosci Methods. 2007 Aug 15;164(1):177-90
pubmed: 17517438
Neuroimage. 2011 May 15;56(2):455-75
pubmed: 20656037
Int J Psychophysiol. 2013 Aug;89(2):252-8
pubmed: 23403325
Dev Sci. 2019 Mar;22(2):e12743
pubmed: 30160012
Neuroimage. 2004;23 Suppl 1:S250-63
pubmed: 15501095
J Neurosci. 2005 Oct 12;25(41):9398-405
pubmed: 16221848
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009 Feb 3;106(5):1608-13
pubmed: 19164756
Trends Neurosci. 1999 Aug;22(8):337-45
pubmed: 10407416
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2022 Aug;7(8):782-792
pubmed: 34655804
Neuroimage. 2020 Dec;223:117361
pubmed: 32919055
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018 Sep 25;115(39):E9220-E9229
pubmed: 30209214
J Neurosci. 2010 Oct 6;30(40):13211-9
pubmed: 20926647
Neuroimage. 2020 Feb 1;206:116331
pubmed: 31712168
Physiol Rev. 1988 Jul;68(3):649-742
pubmed: 2839857
Nat Commun. 2017 Jun 26;8:15930
pubmed: 28649997
J Neurosci. 1995 Jan;15(1 Pt 2):604-22
pubmed: 7823167
Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014 Apr;15(4):264-78
pubmed: 24569488
Clin Neurophysiol. 2003 Nov;114(11):2138-45
pubmed: 14580612
Psychophysiology. 2021 Aug;58(8):e13829
pubmed: 33951193
Neuroscience. 2019 Mar 15;402:104-115
pubmed: 30615913
Neuron. 2017 Jul 19;95(2):424-435.e6
pubmed: 28689981
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012 Aug;36(7):1718-28
pubmed: 22430027
Nat Neurosci. 2013 Apr;16(4):391-3
pubmed: 23434910
J Neurophysiol. 1995 Mar;73(3):921-2
pubmed: 7608777
Front Biosci. 2003 May 01;8:d878-99
pubmed: 12700074

Auteurs

Ann-Kathrin Joechner (AK)

Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany.

Michael A Hahn (MA)

Department of Psychology, Laboratory for Sleep, Cognition and Consciousness Research, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria.
Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience Salzburg (CCNS), University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria.
Hertie-Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University Medical Center Tuebingen, Tuebingen, Germany.

Georg Gruber (G)

Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria.
The Siesta Group, Vienna, Austria.

Kerstin Hoedlmoser (K)

Department of Psychology, Laboratory for Sleep, Cognition and Consciousness Research, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria.
Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience Salzburg (CCNS), University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria.

Markus Werkle-Bergner (M)

Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany.

Articles similaires

[Redispensing of expensive oral anticancer medicines: a practical application].

Lisanne N van Merendonk, Kübra Akgöl, Bastiaan Nuijen
1.00
Humans Antineoplastic Agents Administration, Oral Drug Costs Counterfeit Drugs

Smoking Cessation and Incident Cardiovascular Disease.

Jun Hwan Cho, Seung Yong Shin, Hoseob Kim et al.
1.00
Humans Male Smoking Cessation Cardiovascular Diseases Female
Humans United States Aged Cross-Sectional Studies Medicare Part C
1.00
Humans Yoga Low Back Pain Female Male

Classifications MeSH